Elsevier

Journal of Affective Disorders

Volume 186, 1 November 2015, Pages 127-133
Journal of Affective Disorders

Research report
Identifying differences between depressed adolescent suicide ideators and attempters

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.06.031Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Suicide is the second leading cause of death among adolescents.

  • Adolescent suicide attempters are characterized by greater anhedonia severity.

  • Depressed suicide attempters do not integrate rewarding experiences.

Abstract

Background

Adolescent depression and suicide are pressing public health concerns, and identifying key differences among suicide ideators and attempters is critical. The goal of the current study is to test whether depressed adolescent suicide attempters report greater anhedonia severity and exhibit aberrant effort–cost computations in the face of uncertainty.

Methods

Depressed adolescents (n=101) ages 13–19 years were administered structured clinical interviews to assess current mental health disorders and a history of suicidality (suicide ideators=55, suicide attempters=46). Then, participants completed self-report instruments assessing symptoms of suicidal ideation, depression, anhedonia, and anxiety as well as a computerized effort–cost computation task.

Results

Compared with depressed adolescent suicide ideators, attempters report greater anhedonia severity, even after concurrently controlling for symptoms of suicidal ideation, depression, and anxiety. Additionally, when completing the effort–cost computation task, suicide attempters are less likely to pursue the difficult, high value option when outcomes are uncertain. Follow-up, trial-level analyses of effort–cost computations suggest that receipt of reward does not influence future decision-making among suicide attempters, however, suicide ideators exhibit a win–stay approach when receiving rewards on previous trials.

Limitations

Findings should be considered in light of limitations including a modest sample size, which limits generalizability, and the cross-sectional design.

Conclusions

Depressed adolescent suicide attempters are characterized by greater anhedonia severity, which may impair the ability to integrate previous rewarding experiences to inform future decisions. Taken together, this may generate a feeling of powerlessness that contributes to increased suicidality and a needless loss of life.

Introduction

Adolescent depression and suicide are serious public health concerns. The lifetime prevalence of depression in adolescence is 11% (Avenevoli et al., 2015), and depressive episodes are associated with poor psychosocial functioning (e.g., greater discord with parents, lower academic performance), more frequent engagement in risky behaviors (e.g., substance use, precocious sexual behaviors), and suicidality (Auerbach et al., 2010, Frojd et al., 2008). Presently, suicide is the second leading cause of death among youth ages 13–18 (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2013), and depressed adolescents have a 6-fold greater likelihood of making a suicide attempt (Nock et al., 2013). Nevertheless, the explicit link between depression and suicidality remains unclear. For adolescents, depression is among the strongest predictors of suicidal ideation and plans but shows a weaker relationship with suicide attempts, and critically, only a third of adolescent ideators make an attempt (Nock et al., 2013). However, few prior studies have examined potential mechanisms that can explain how depression contributes to the transition from ideation to attempt.

Both depression and suicide are characterized by diminished motivation and deficits in reward processing (Auerbach et al., 2014a, Dombrovski et al., 2013). In animal studies, dysfunction within the mesocorticolimbic pathway contributes to reduced social drive and reward pursuit, which has been depicted as a depression-like behavior (Mintz et al., 2005, Strekalova et al., 2004). Among humans, anhedonia – a reduced ability to experience pleasure – is a core feature in depression (Pizzagalli et al., 2005), and both depressed adolescents (Boger et al., 2014, Forbes et al., 2009) and adults (Bogdan and Pizzagalli, 2006, Pizzagalli et al., 2008) exhibit reward dysfunction, which may be a promising phenotype of depression. Converging evidence from neuroimaging studies characterizing the neuropathology of these reward deficits has highlighted dysfunction within the anterior cingulate cortex (Steele et al., 2007) and striatum (Pizzagalli et al., 2009). Less research has examined behavioral and neural correlates of anhedonia in suicide, although studies have revealed that suicide attempters show aberrant reward signaling (Dombrovski et al., 2013) and abnormal striatum structure (Ahearn et al., 2001, Vang et al., 2010), compared with non-suicidal individuals.

Indeed, anhedonia may be a promising predictor of suicidal behavior, as it has long been theorized that suicide is a decision to escape from intolerable conditions when there is no expectation of relief (Baumeister, 1990, Hawton et al., 1982, Nock and Kazdin, 2002). Along these lines, Nock and Kazdin (2002) posit that for many, the experience of anhedonia is insufferable, especially because it tends to be chronic and stable over time (Clark et al., 1984). Although there seem to be clear theoretical connections between anhedonia and suicidal behaviors, results have been equivocal. In adults, anhedonia predicted suicide completions in severe psychiatric patients within 1 year of assessment (Fawcett et al., 1990). These findings were largely replicated in an independent sample: anhedonia was present in early (i.e., 1 year of the assessment) but not late (after 1 year of the assessment) suicide completions (Coryell and Young, 2005). Other research, however, found that anhedonia did not prospectively predict suicide attempts. Rather, there was a stronger relationship between low anhedonia and increased likelihood of suicidal behaviors, and the researchers suggested that the presence of high energy and elevated emotion might be a necessary condition for suicidality (Loas, 2007, Watson and Kucala, 1978).

Comparatively less research, however, has been conducted in youth. In a study of children ages 6–13 admitted to an acute psychiatric facility, anhedonia was associated with the presence of a suicide attempt even after controlling for depressed mood. Moreover, greater anhedonia severity distinguished psychiatric inpatient suicide attempters and ideators (Nock and Kazdin, 2002). Similarly, in a study of adolescents, higher levels of anhedonia distinguished youth reporting a history of both suicide attempts and nonsuicidal self-injury from those with no suicide attempt history and current nonsuicidal self-injury (Brausch and Gutierrez, 2010). Yet, the study did not account for the potential influence of current depressive symptoms or suicidal ideation, which makes it challenging to ascertain the unique effect of anhedonia on suicide attempt status. By contrast, another adolescent study found that greater anhedonia was correlated with suicidal ideation but not intent (i.e., suicide plan); the study did not assess the occurrence of suicide attempts (Chabrol et al., 2007). Although this research is a promising start to parsing the effect of anhedonia on suicide attempts in youth, important gaps remain, particularly in understanding how anhedonia may contribute to suicide attempts among depressed adolescents.

Anhedonia impairs the ability to integrate rewarding experiences (Pizzagalli et al., 2005), which is essential for informing future decisions, and consequently, this might be one path that leads to attempting suicide. Specifically, as anhedonia is characterized by marked diminishment in pleasure in previously enjoyed activities (Pizzagalli et al., 2008), these experiences are not viewed as positive, and therefore, they are unlikely to increase the likelihood of engaging in them in the future. A possible downstream consequence is that anhedonic individuals tend to make abnormal effort–cost computations. Meaning, they avoid previously enjoyed activities as they overestimate the cost (i.e., effort expenditure) and underestimate the benefit (i.e., future reward) (Gold et al., 2012, Gold et al., 2013) – especially when the perceived benefit is uncertain – which reduces engagement in pursuing rewards across social (e.g., spending time with friends) and experiential (e.g., playing sports) domains. Over time, this decreases the opportunity to experience pleasure, which may contribute to the chronicity of anhedonia and increased suicidality.

The goal of the current study is to test whether depressed adolescent suicide attempters show increased anhedonic behaviors compared with ideators that have comparable symptom severity across depression, anxiety, and suicidal ideation. As the two groups do not differ on key clinical variables, any resulting differences cannot be attributed to symptom severity. We tested the following a priori hypotheses. First, we hypothesized that relative to ideators, suicide attempters would report greater levels of anhedonia even after controlling for current symptom severity. Second, for individuals experiencing anhedonia, the value of future rewards may not offer sufficient motivation to pursue goal-directed behaviors, particularly when: (a) the reward is uncertain and (b) the costs (i.e., effort expenditure) to obtain the rewards are perceived as outweighing the benefits (Gold et al., 2013). This abnormal effort–cost computation may be more pronounced in suicide attempters reporting more severe anhedonia, and thus, we hypothesized that compared to suicide ideators, attempters will be less likely to pursue uncertain, but not certain, rewards. Last, using a fine-grained approach to examine the effort–cost computation deficit, trial-level data analysis will demonstrate that suicide ideators and attempters differ in their behavior following receipt of rewards. Specifically, we hypothesize that for suicide attempters, reward receipt will not influence the pursuit of future rewards. By contrast, suicide ideators will be more likely to exhibit a win–stay approach – repeat the selection of an option after receiving a reward.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants included 101 adolescents (82 female, 19 male) ages 13–19 years (M=15.50, SD=1.48) recruited upon admission to a short-term, acute residential psychiatric facility. Inclusion criteria included a primary diagnosis of major depressive disorder or dysthymia and the presence of suicidal ideation (i.e.,≥4 on Beck's Scale for Suicide Ideation (Beck et al., 1979). Exclusion criteria included a history of hypomania/mania or psychosis. Based on results of the suicide assessment (i.e.,

Examination of group differences

Means, standard deviations, and results from the independent samples t-tests for suicidal ideation, depressive symptoms, anhedonia, and anxious symptoms are reported in Table 1, and partial correlations for baseline self-report instruments while controlling for current depressive symptoms is included in Table 2. In line with our hypothesis, suicide attempters reported significantly greater levels of anhedonia, and this difference remained after concurrently controlling for depressive symptoms,

Discussion

The alarming prevalence rates of depression and suicidality in adolescents underscore the enormity of the present public health concern (Avenevoli et al., 2015, Nock et al., 2013). To our knowledge, the study is the first to report key differences in abnormal effort–cost computations in depressed adolescent suicide ideators and attempters with comparable symptom severity. Results indicated that compared to depressed suicide ideators, depressed suicide attempters reported greater anhedonia

Limitations

It is important to note several limitations in our approach. First, the study included a relatively modest sample size (n=101), which may impact the generalizability of these findings. Second, the current study examined suicide in the context of depression; however, the pathway to suicide is equifinal and occurs in the context of an array of disorders (e.g., bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder) (Nock et al., 2013). Therefore, future research is needed to determine whether

Conclusions

Adolescent suicide attempters are characterized by greater anhedonia severity, which may contribute to an inability to integrate previous rewarding experiences that, ideally, should inform future decision-making. These results highlight the importance of identifying key differences among suicide ideators and attempters, as this will, ultimately, better inform the development of suicide models that improve identification of: (a) ideators at greatest risk to make a suicide attempt and (b)

Conflict of interest

None.

Role of funding source

Dr. Auerbach was partially supported through funding from: NIMH K23MH097786, the Kaplen Fellowship on Depression awarded by Harvard Medical School, the Tommy Fuss Fund, and the Simches Fund. Dr. Stewart was supported, in part, through the Skip Pope Award for Young Investigators awarded by McLean Hospital. The funding sources did not assist with study design, data acquisition, or data analysis. Manuscript preparation and submission was managed by the authors.

Contributions

We confirm that the final manuscript has been read and approved by all named authors and that there are no other persons who satisfied criteria for authorship but are not listed. We further confirm that the order of authors listed in the manuscript has been approved by all of us.

With respect to roles in the project and manuscript, Dr. Auerbach designed the study, completed preliminary analyses, and prepared the majority of the manuscript. Mr. Millner also assisted with the study

Acknowledgments

The authors are appreciative of the ongoing support and collaboration with the staff from the Adolescent Residential Treatment program at McLean Hospital.

References (40)

  • E.P. Ahearn et al.

    MRI correlates of suicide attempt history in unipolar depression

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • R.P. Auerbach et al.

    Adolescent depression: stress and reward dysfunction

    Harv. Rev. Psychiatry

    (2014)
  • R.P. Auerbach et al.

    Adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury: examining the role of child abuse, comorbidity, and disinhibition

    Psychiatry Res.

    (2014)
  • R.P. Auerbach et al.

    Temporal relationships among depressive symptoms, risky behavior engagement, perceived control, and gender in a sample of adolescents

    J. Res. Adolesc.

    (2010)
  • S. Avenevoli et al.

    Major depression in the national comorbidity survey-adolescent supplement: prevalence, correlates, and treatment

    J. Am. Acad. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry

    (2015)
  • R.F. Baumeister

    Suicide as escape from self

    Psychol. Rev.

    (1990)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Assessment of suicidal intention: the scale for suicide ideation

    J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.

    (1979)
  • R. Bogdan et al.

    Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: implications for depression

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • K.D. Boger et al.

    Co-occurring depressive and substance use disorders in adolescents: an examination of reward responsiveness during treatment

    J. Psychother. Integr.

    (2014)
  • A.M. Brausch et al.

    Differences in non-suicidal self-injury and suicide attempts in adolescents

    J. Youth Adolesc.

    (2010)
  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2013. Web-based injury statistics query and reporting system, National...
  • H. Chabrol et al.

    Relations between suicidal ideation and dimensions of depressive symptoms in high-school students

    J. Adolesc.

    (2007)
  • D.C. Clark et al.

    Seven-month clinical outcome of anhedonic and normally hedonic depressed inpatients

    Am. J. Psychiatry

    (1984)
  • W. Coryell et al.

    Clinical predictors of suicide in primary major depressive disorder

    J. Clin. Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • A.Y. Dombrovski et al.

    Reward signals, attempted suicide, and impulsivity in late-life depression

    JAMA Psychiatry

    (2013)
  • J. Fawcett et al.

    Time-related predictors of suicide in major affective disorder

    Am. J. Psychiatry

    (1990)
  • E.E. Forbes et al.

    Genetic variation in components of dopamine neurotransmission impacts ventral striatal reactivity associated with impulsivity

    Mol. Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • S.A. Frojd et al.

    Depression and school performance in middle adolescent boys and girls

    J. Adolesc.

    (2008)
  • J.M. Gold et al.

    Negative symptoms of schizophrenia are associated with abnormal effort-cost computations

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2013)
  • J.M. Gold et al.

    Negative symptoms and the failure to represent the expected reward value of actions: behavioral and computational modeling evidence

    Arch. Gen. Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • Cited by (93)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text